SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.17 número1Evidencia fósil y probable extinción del murciélago pescador Noctilio leporinus (Chiroptera: Noctilionidae) en Isla de Mona, Puerto RicoActualización sobre el estado del apostadero de lobo marino de un pelo sudamericano (Otaria Flavescens) en la Isla Trinidad, estuario de Bahía Blanca, Argentina índice de autoresíndice de materiabúsqueda de artículos
Home Pagelista alfabética de revistas  

Servicios Personalizados

Revista

Articulo

Indicadores

  • No hay articulos citadosCitado por SciELO

Links relacionados

Compartir


Mastozoología neotropical

versión impresa ISSN 0327-9383versión On-line ISSN 1666-0536

Mastozool. neotrop. v.17 n.1 Mendoza ene./jun. 2010

 

ARTÍCULOS Y NOTAS

Adoption of an orphan infant in wild black and gold howler monkeys (Alouatta caraya)

Romina Pavé1, Martín M. Kowalewski1,2, Gabriel E. Zunino1

1 Estación Biológica Corrientes, Museo Argentino de Ciencias Naturales "B. Rivadavia", Ruta Pcial. 8 s/n, 3401 San Cayetano, Corrientes, Argentina [Corresponding author: Romina Pavé <rominaepave@yahoo.com.ar>].
2 Department of Anthropology, University of Illinois at Urbana- Champaign, Urbana, Illinois.

ABSTRACT: This study reports the first case of adoption in a wild group of black and gold howler monkeys (Alouatta caraya). After the death of one adult female with a dependent infant, her daughter with a dependent infant adopted the orphan of similar age of her own infant. We recorded the activity and proximity between the adopter and both infants during two following days. The adoptive female appeared to provide equal treatment toward both infants and the orphan seemed to recognize the female as his mother. This report suggests that adult females of A. caraya can rear two infants at once.

RESUMEN: Adopción de un infante huérfano en monos aulladores negros y dorados (Alouatta caraya). En este estudio se cita el primer caso de adopción en un grupo silvestre de monos aulladores negros y dorados (Alouatta caraya). Luego de la muerte de una hembra adulta, su hija adoptó al infante huérfano de similar edad que su propio infante. Durante dos días seguidos registramos la actividad y proximidad de la hembra adoptiva y los dos infantes. La hembra adoptiva expresó igual trato hacia ambos infantes y el infante huérfano parecía reconocer a la hembra como su propia madre. Este estudio sugiere que hembras adultas de A. caraya pueden criar dos infantes a la vez.

Key words. Adoption; Alloparental care; Alouatta caraya; Kin selection; Orphans.

Palabras clave. Adopción; Alouatta caraya; Cuidado aloparental; Huérfano; Selección por parentesco.

Infant adoption is a permanent association between a lactating female and a nutritionally dependent infant other than her offspring (Maestripieri, 2001). Several factors influence both the likelihood and the permanence of adoption, including age, sex, kinship, ecological conditions, and in the case of adoptive females, reproductive status (Thierry and Anderson, 1986). Adoption may be interpreted as a cooperative behavior explained by kinselection if relatives adopt the infant (West-Eberhard, 1975) or reciprocal altruism if the adoptee provides future social support for the adoptive female (Trivers, 1971; West Eberhard, 1975). However adoption can be interpreted as a selfish behavior of the adoptive mother if she uses this opportunity either to practice maternal skills (Lancaster, 1971) or to reduce the fitness of rival females (Silk, 1980).
Adoption of infants has been reported in several mammalian species including non-human primates (Riedman, 1982; Thierry and Anderson, 1986). In the genus Alouatta, adoption has been documented in A. palliata (Clarke and Glander, 1981), A. pigra (Schneider et al., 1999), and A. seniculus (Figueroa, 1989; Izawa, 1989; Agoramoorthy and Rudran, 1992). In this study we document the first case of adoption reported for wild black and gold howler monkeys (Alouatta caraya). A. caraya, like other species of howler monkeys, is a folivore-frugivore arboreal and diurnal primate characterized by bisexual dispersal (Crockett and Eisenberg, 1987; Di Fiore and Campbell, 2007). In the case of A. caraya the adults are sexually dichromatic and sexually dimorphic (Di Fiore and Campbell, 2007).
The study site is Isla Brasilera (27° 18' S; 58° 38' W) in Northern Argentina characterized by a continuous flooded forest and located near the confluence of the rivers Paraná and Paraguay (Kowalewski and Zunino, 2004). A more complete description of the site is provided in Kowalewski and Zunino (2004). The case of adoption was observed on September 12, 2008. Prior to the adoption in August, 2008, the study group had 12 individuals (one adult male, three adult females, two subadult females, one juvenile female, three juvenile males, and two infants). This group was continuously studied since 2001 (Kowalewski and Zunino, 2004; Kowalewski, 2007; Oklander, 2007; Peker et al., 2008) and individuals were identified by body size, pelage colors, and natural and artificial marks. The maternal kinship of the infants and juveniles was known (Kowalewski, 2007; Oklander, 2007; Pavé pers. obs.). Two out of three resident adult females (Josefa and Gorda) were mother and daughter respectively. Gorda was a juvenile of less than two years old when behavioral data collection began on this group and there was a strong affiliative association (maternal care, time in contact, and grooming interactions) among them in comparison to other female-female dyads (Ana and Gorda) (Kowalewski, 2007; Oklander, 2007). The two infants belonging to Josefa and Gorda were born in July, 2008. The infant of Josefa was born approximately 10 days before the infant of Gorda. Behavioral observations began the day after finding the remains of Josefa. Focal and instantaneous point samples techniques were employed during two following days. Continuous focal observations on the adoptive female and both infants simultaneously were taken from sunrise to sunset to record the activity and proximity between the three individuals (Altmann, 1974). Instantaneous point samples were taken every five min during the focal sample. We obtained 22.45 h of focal samples and 258 instantaneous point samples.
On September 12, 2008 (15:43) we found the head of an adult female (Josefa) within the home range of the study group. The other female with a dependent offspring (Gorda) was carrying her own infant (i1) and the infant of Josefa (i2). Josefa was a multiparous adult female when she was studied for the first time in 2001 (Oklander, 2007), and we assume that she was at least 13 years old when she died for unknown causes. On September 13, 2008 (06:45) we began to record the behavior in this group. Because both infants were males of similar age (approximately two months old) and size, it was not possible to distinguish them most of the time.
Gorda was observed during 59.7 % (N=154) of the instantaneous point samples in contact with both infants when she was resting, feeding, moving, and grooming the infants. In the remaining 40.3% (N=104) of the time the infants were feeding, moving, exploring, playing together, and interacting with other group members. During feeding, Gorda carried both infants (in dorsal, ventral, and lateral position) or the infants were feeding, exploring, and playing between 0 and 1 m distance from her. During moving, Gorda carried both infants (in dorsal, ventral, and lateral position) and just on two occasions, she carried one infant while the other one followed her (Fig. 1). In addition, Gorda spent 18.6% (N=48) of the instantaneous point samples (or 3.3hs of observation) nursing the infants and on 18 occasions during the focal, she simultaneously nursed both infants. On seven occasions during the focal the infants fought (screamed and tried to move the other away from the nipple) when they attempted to suckle on the same nipple.


Fig. 1. The adopter female moving with both infants in dorsal position (Photo by R. Pavé).

On one occasion during the focal in which i2 was suckling, Gorda pushed her own infant (i1) away when it attempted to suckling.
One or both infants interacted with the other group members and this accounted for 4.65% (N=12) of the instantaneous point samples. The interactions included: dorsal carrying, grooming the infants, playing with infants, and touching and biting the infants. Dorsal carrying was observed just on one occasion when the subadult female Ema (non-relative to Gorda) carried one infant that stayed behind when Gorda moved away to feed. The individuals who groomed the infants were juvenile and subadult females. On four occasions during the focal, Gorda behave as a restrictive mother either by placing her body between the infant and her three year old daughter Charlie or subadult Ema or by retrieving the infant from them.
Spontaneous adoptions in non-human primates both in wild and captive groups suggest that neonates are most likely to be adopted by adult females who had recently lost their infants or lactating females that adopt an additional infant and rear "twins" (i.e. Thierry and Anderson, 1986; Izawa, 1989; Gould, 2000; Casar and Young, 2008). Thierry and Anderson (1986) suggested that hormonal conditions associated with late pregnancy and lactation appear to potentiate adoptive tendencies in adult females. Moreover, several studies reported that infants less than three months old had been adopted or kidnapped by adult individuals (Agoramoorthy and Rudran, 1992; Agoramoorthy, 1998; Biedzicki de Marques and Ades, 2000; Gould, 2000).
In this study, the adoptive female expressed the general adoptive behavioral patterns suggested by Thierry and Anderson (1986): nursing, carrying and cuddling, and protecting. Because she had an infant of her own, she simultaneously carried and nursed both infants. Agoramoorthy (1998) reports a case of a female of A. seniculus simultaneously carrying her own twins as well as an alien infant. The adopter appeared to provide equal treatment toward both infants and she protected both when other individuals attempted to take the infants away from her. Similarly, the adoptee seemed to recognize the adult female as his mother. This observation coincides with such cited in Thierry and Anderson (1986).
This adoption cannot be attributed to gaining parental experience (Lancaster, 1971) or reducing the fitness of rival females (Silk, 1980) because the adoptive female (Gorda) was multiparous and the biological mother of the infant (Josefa) died. We suggest that this adoption event may be explained by kin selection (West-Eberhard, 1975). Adoption is possibly a costly behavior because it causes the adoptive female to concurrently invest in an extra infant; however a female adopting her young brother may increase her inclusive fitness if both infants survive. In this regard we suggest that the benefits obtained through the adoption of a relative outweigh the costs of investing in rearing an extra infant. Examples of adoption of relatives, although rare, have been reported for different species of nonhuman primates such as Alouatta seniculus (Izawa, 1989; Agoramoorthy and Rudran, 1992), Lemur catta (Gould, 2000), Presbytis entellus (Dolhinow and DeMay, 1982), and Saimiri sciureus (Scollay, 1978).
This study shows that adult females of A. caraya like other Alouatta species can adopt infants and rear more than one infant at once. The adopted infant in our study was in June 2009 eleven months old and both adopter and adoptee seem to behave as biological mother and offspring. Finally, we ignore if the reproductive cost of raising two young infants is high to affect the interbirth interval (IBI) of the female. Agoramoorthy and Rudran (1992) reported an increase of five months in the IBI of a female who adopted an extra infant. In this regard, we suggest that the cost of raising two infants may be overridden by the benefits that this investment produces.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We thank to Juan Bartoli for logistic support. We also thank Amparo Perez Rueda for field assistance. We thank Melissa Raguet for valuable comments on an earlier version of this manuscript. The comments and suggestions of the anonymous reviewer significantly improved this article. This study was funded by CONICET, the American Society of Mammalogists, and Idea Wild.

LITERATURE CITED

1. AGORAMOORTHY G. 1998. Intergroup infant transfer among red howler, Alouatta seniculus, in Venezuela: adoption or kidnapping? Neotropical Primates 6:121-123.         [ Links ]

2. AGORAMOORTHY G and R RUDRAN. 1992. Adoption in free-ranging red howler monkeys, Alouatta seniculus of Venezuela. Primates 33:551-555.         [ Links ]

3. ALTMANN J. 1974. Observational study of behavior: Sampling methods. Behaviour 49:227-267.         [ Links ]

4. BIEDZICKI DE MARQUES AA and C ADES. 2000. Male care in a group of wild Alouatta fusca clamitans in Southern Brazil. Folia Primatologica 71:409-412.         [ Links ]

5. CASAR C and RJ YOUNG. 2008. A case of adoption in a wild group of black-fronted titi monkeys (Callicebus nigrifrons). Primates 49:146-148.         [ Links ]

6. CLARKE MR and KE GLANDER. 1981. Adoption of infant howling monkeys (Alouatta palliata). American Journal of Primatology 1:469-472.         [ Links ]

7. CROCKETT CM and JF EISENBERG. 1987. Howlers: variations in group size and demography. Pp 54-68, in: Primate Societies (BB Smuts, DL Cheney, RM Seyfarth, RW Wrangham, and TT Struhsaker, eds.). The University of Chicago Press, Chicago.         [ Links ]

8. DI FIORE A and CJ CAMPBELL. 2007. The Atelines: variation in ecology, behavior, and social organization. Pp 155-185, in: Primates in Perspective (CJ Campbell, A Fuentes, KC MacKinnon, M Panger, and SK Bearder, eds.). Oxford University Pres, New York.         [ Links ]

9. DOLHINOW P and MG DEMAY. 1982. Adoption: The importance of infant choice. Journal of Human Evolution 11:391-420.         [ Links ]

10. FIGUEROA R. 1989. Social interactions of a fourth month adopted infant in a wild group of Alouatta seniculus. Field studies of New World Monkeys, La Macarena, Colombia 2:37-39.         [ Links ]

11. GOULD L. 2000. Adoption of a wild orphaned ringtailed lemur infant by natal group members: Adaptive explanations. Primates 41:413-419.         [ Links ]

12. IZAWA K. 1989. The adoption of an infant observed in a wild group of red howler monkeys (Alouatta seniculus). Field Studies of New World Monkeys, La Macarena, Columbia 2:33-36.         [ Links ]

13. KOWALEWSKI MM. 2007. Patterns of affiliation and co-operation in howler monkeys: an alternative model to explain social organization in non-human primates. Doctoral thesis. University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign, USA. AAT 3290280.         [ Links ]

14. KOWALEWSKI MM and GE ZUNINO. 2004. Birth seasonality in Alouatta caraya in northern Argentina. International Journal of Primatology 25:383-400.         [ Links ]

15. LANCASTER JB. 1971. Play-mothering: the relations between juvenile females and young infants among free ranging vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). Folia Primatologica 15:161-182.         [ Links ]

16. MAESTRIPIERI D. 2001. Is there mother-infant bonding in Primates? Developmental Review 21:93-120.         [ Links ]

17. OKLANDER LI. 2007. Estructura social y relaciones de parentesco en poblaciones silvestres de monos aulladores (Alouatta caraya) del noreste argentino. Doctoral Thesis, Universidad de Buenos Aires, Buenos Aires, Argentina.         [ Links ]

18. PEKER SM, MM KOWALEWSKI, R PAVE, and GE ZUNINO. 2008. Births in wild black and gold howler monkeys (Alouatta caraya) in northern Argentina. American Journal of Primatology 70:1-5.         [ Links ]

19. RIEDMAN ML. 1982. The evolution of alloparental care and adoption in mammals and birds. The Quarterly Review of Biology 57:405-435.         [ Links ]

20. SCHNEIDER EC, LF HUNTER, and RH HORWICH. 1999. Adoption of a young juvenile in black howler monkeys (Alouatta pigra). Neotropical Primates 7:47-51.         [ Links ]

21. SCOLLAY PA. 1978. The kidnapping of a neonate squirrel monkey Saimiri sciureus (Peruvian). Laboratory Primate Newsletter 17:11-13.         [ Links ]

22. SILK JB. 1980. Kidnapping and female competition among captive bonnet macaques. Primates 21:100-110.         [ Links ]

23. THIERRY B and JR ANDERSON. 1986. Adoption in Anthropoid Primates. International Journal of Primatology 7:191-216.         [ Links ]

24. TRIVERS RL. 1971. The evolution of reciprocal altruism. The Quarterly Review of Biology 46:35-57.         [ Links ]

25. WEST-EBERHARD MJ. 1975. The evolution of social behavior by kin selection. The Quarterly Review of Biology 50:1-33.         [ Links ]

Recibido 3 diciembre 2009.
Aceptado 15 febrero 2010.

Editor asociado: U Pardiñas

Creative Commons License Todo el contenido de esta revista, excepto dónde está identificado, está bajo una Licencia Creative Commons