SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.7 número2Flow rate of polygonal grains through a bottleneck: Interplay between shape and sizeBayesian regression of piecewise homogeneous Poisson processes índice de autoresíndice de materiabúsqueda de artículos
Home Pagelista alfabética de revistas  

Servicios Personalizados

Revista

Articulo

Indicadores

  • No hay articulos citadosCitado por SciELO

Links relacionados

  • No hay articulos similaresSimilares en SciELO

Compartir


Papers in physics

versión On-line ISSN 1852-4249

Pap. Phys. vol.7 no.2 La Plata dic. 2015

 

 

How we move is universal: Scaling in the average shape of human activity

 

Dante R. Chialvo,1 Ana Maria Gonzalez Torrado,2 Ewa Gudowska-Nowak,3 Jeremi K. Ochab,4 Pedro Montoya,2 Maciej A. Nowak,3'4 Enzo Tagliazucchi5

Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Tecnológicas (CONICET), Rivadavia 1917, Buenos Aires, Argentina.

Institut Universitari d'Investigacions en Ciencies de la Salut (IUNICS) & Universitat de les Illes Balears (UIB), Palma de Mallorca, Spain.

M. Kac Complex Systems Research Center and M. Smoluchowski Institute of Physics, Jagiellonian University, Krakóow, Poland.

Biocomplexity Department, Malopolska Center of Biotechnology, Jagiellonian University, Krakóow, Poland.

Institute for Medical Psychology, Christian Albrechts University, Kiel, Germany.


Human motor activity is constrained by the rhythmicity of the 24 hours circadian cycle, including the usual 12-15 hours sleep-wake cycle. However, activity fluctuations also appear over a wide range of temporal scales, from days to a few seconds, resulting from the concatenation of a myriad of individual smaller motor events. Furthermore, individuals present different propensity to wakefulness and thus to motor activity throughout the circadian cycle. Are activity fluctuations across temporal scales intrinsically different, or is there a universal description encompassing them? Is this description also universal across individuals, considering the aforementioned variability? Here we establish the presence of universality in motor activity fluctuations based on the empirical study of a month of continuous wristwatch accelerometer recordings. We study the scaling of average fluctuations across temporal scales and determine a universal law characterized by critical exponents a, t and 1//i. Results are highly reminiscent of the universality described for the average shape of avalanches in systems exhibiting crackling noise. Beyond its theoretical relevance, the present results can be important for developing objective markers of healthy as well as pathological human motor behavior.

Keywords Circadian Cycle; Motor Activity


 

I. Introduction

The most obvious periodicity of human (as well as animal) motor activity is the circadian twenty four hours modulation. However, smaller fluctuations are evident on a wide range of temporal scales, from days to a few seconds. Data shows that the activity evolves in bursts of all sizes and durations which are known to be scale-invariant 1-8 regardless of the origins and intended consequences of such activity. Despite the variety of results, the mechanisms underlying the scale-invariant behavior of motor activity remain to be elucidated. Considering the intermittent nature of human motor activity - comprising brief activity excursions separated by periods of quiescence - a natural approach would be to study the average shape of the events, following recent results 9-12 which show that for a large class of processes, the average shape is a scaling function determined mostly by the temporal correlations of the process and its nonlinearities 13.

In the present work, long time series of human motor activity are analyzed, recorded via wrist-watch accelerometer, lasting approximately one month. We establish first the presence of truncated scale-invariance in the distribution of the durations of the events as well as in its power spectral density, as described previously in similar type of data. Afterwards, we uncover the average shape of the bursts of activity and derive the scaling function and its associated exponents. Finally, we discuss the origins of such scaling and some possible applications.

II. Materials and methods

The recordings analyzed were part of a larger study and included six healthy, non-smokers, drug-free volunteers (mean age 50.1 years, S.D. = 6.8). The study was approved by the Bioethics Commission of the University of Isles Baleares (Spain). Participants were informed about the procedures and goals of the study, and provided their written consent. After determining their handedness, each subject was provided with a wristwatch-sized activity recorder (Actiwatch from Mini- Mitter Co., OR, USA) measuring acceleration changes in the forearm in any plane. Each data point of activity corresponded to the number of zero crossings in acceleration larger than 0.01 G (sampled at 32 Hz and integrated over a 30-second window length). Records of several thousands of data points were kept in the device's internal memory until being downloaded to a personal computer every week. Subjects wore the device in their non-dominant arm continuously for up to several weeks (mean 28.1 days, S.D.= 4.). After careful visual inspection of the data to exclude sets with gaps (due to subject non-compliance), a combined total of 280 days of data was available for further analysis.

III. Results

For ease of presentation, we will use recordings from a single subject to describe the main results. Nevertheless, results are robust as well as similar for the entire group of subjects in the study. A typical recording is presented in Fig. 1. Panel A shows a full month of continuously recorded activity from this subject, who is particularly regular in her daily routines. The subject wakes up with the alarm clock at 6:45 a.m. on week days and has lunch followed by a short nap each day (between 2:00 p.m. and 4:00 p.m. Panel B displays the time series of the successive increments of the signal x(n), defined as I(n) = x(n +1) — x(n).

 


Figure 1: Example data set, distribution of successive increments and their spectral power. Panel A: Time series of activity x(n) recorded continuously from a subject during a month. Individual traces correspond to consecutive days. The top subpanel depicts daily activity averaged over the entire month. Panel B: Time series of successive increments I(n) = x(n +1) - x(n) (normalized by its SD) for the same data. Panel C: Probability density distribution of the time series of successive increments I( n) (continuous line), exhibiting exponential tails (compare with the dotted line, a Gaussian of the same variance). Panel D: Power spectral density (black line) of the time series of successive increments I( n) of panel B. This is scale invariant S(f) ~ f7 with 7 = 0.9 (dashed line). In contrast, for the randomly shuffled increments, the serial correlations vanish and a flat spectral density is obtained (red).


Figure 2: Scaling of activity events in a single subject (same dataset as in Fig. 1). Panel A: The complementary cumulative distribution function (CCDF) for event durations (T) and sizes (S) obeys power-laws with exponents a' = 0.70 and t' = 0.44, respectively (dashed lines). Note that here the densities are cumulative, thus the exponents of the respective PDFs are a = a' + 1 and t = t' + 1. The waiting time between events falls exponentially. Panel B: The average size of a given duration is well described (for small T) by (S)(T) - Twith // +1 = 1.59 (blue dashed line) comparable with results obtained from fitting within the scaling region (red filled symbols) giving // + 1 = 1.61.

The large-scale statistical features of the time series presented in Fig. 1 are already well known. The density distribution of the successive increments «(n) is non-Gaussian, as can be appreciated by a joint plot with a Gaussian distribution of the same variance (Fig. 1, Panel C). It is known that the power spectrum of the activity decays as S(f) ~ f ^ 1,2. Because this type of processes are likely to be non-stationary, it is best to estimate the exponents of the spectral density by doing the calculations over the time series of successive increments, whose density distribution is stationary. For instance, for Brownian motion (which is summed white noise), the power spectrum decays S(f) ~ f ^ with /3 = -2 and for white noise /3 = 0; the summed time series has an exponent +2 larger than the non-summed time series. As discussed in 14, this can be generalized for all self-affine processes: summing a self-affine time series shifts the theoretical power-spectral density exponent by +2, and the reverse process is also true: the differences in consecutive values (the "first differences") of a Brownian motion result in white noise, thus taking the first differences shifts the theoretical power-spectral density exponent / by -2. In our case, the exponent obtained for the time series of successive increments I( n) was 7 = 0. 9. Thus, the exponent of the raw data is / = 7 - 2 = - 1. 1 14. For comparison, the spectral densities of the actual signal and of a surrogate obtained after randomly shuffling the increments are jointly displayed in Panel D of Fig. 1.

To further study the time series from the perspective of individual bursts of activity, we introduce the definition of an event. We consider the time series of activity x( n) and select a threshold value U to be vanishingly small. An event is defined by the consecutive points starting when x(n) > U and ending when x(n) < U. This is equivalent to the definition of avalanches in other contexts 9,15. In the following part, we will be concerned primarily with the statistics of event lifetimes T, as well as of their average size S and shape. In all subjects, we found that the distributions of event durations and sizes (defined by the area, i. e., the integral of the signal corresponding to the individual events) can be well described, for relatively small values, by a power-law (Fig. 2, Panel A). In contrast, the distribution of waiting times between events demonstrated an exponential decay. In addition to the scale invariance, we found that the longer an event lasted, the stronger the motor activity executed by the subject. The plot of average event size (S) as a function of duration T follows a power-law (for small values of T) described by (S)(T) = Twith /j, + 1 = 1.59. The exponents in this power-law are robust across subjects and to changes of threshold over a reasonable range of values.


Figure 3: Collapse of events of different duration into a single functional form. Panel A: Three examples of typical events of duration T=480, 960 and 1920 sec.. Panel B: The heterogeneous events shown in Panel A can be collapsed onto the average shape (dashed black line) by normalizing t to t/T and (x(t)) to (x(t))/T^. The inset shows the cumulative variance for a range of j. Panel C: The average event shape, i.e., fshape(t/T), recovered from six data sets (thin lines). The best fit using an inverted parabola is shown as a red dashed line (j = 0.49) as well as the one expected from the critical exponent j = 0.59 as a dot-dashed blue line.

This type of scaling is well known in the statistical mechanics of critical phenomena 15. Examples range from earthquakes 16 to active transport processes in cells 17, crackling noise 11, the statistics of Barkhausen noise in permalloy thin films 10 and plastic deformation of metals 18. In all these cases, the distributions obey universal functional forms:

where f denotes the probability density functions of the size of the event S and its duration T, and ( S) ( T) is the expected size for a given duration. The parameters t , a and are the critical exponents of the system and are expected to be independent of the details, being related to each other by the scaling relation:

We found that the empirical exponents very closely fulfill the expression above. Using the fitting approach introduced by Clauset 19 in the scaling regions depicted in Panel A of Fig. 2, we found t =1.44 and a = 1.70 . Thus, from Eq. (4) a value of 1/<rvz = j +1 = 1.59 is expected. The experimental data points are very close to this theoretical expectation (dashed line), especially for the relatively small T values within the scaling region of Panel A (where a linear fit estimates j + 1 = 1.61), while those for relatively larger T values (corresponding to the cutoff of the distributions) are a bit apart, probably due to undersampling. After repeating this analysis for all subjects in our sample, the average exponents were all within 5% of the reported values.

From scaling arguments, it is expected that the average shape of an event of duration T (x(T, t)) scales as :

Thus, the shapes of events of different durations T rescaled by /x should collapse on a single scaling function given by fshape(t/T). Note that // corresponds in this context to the wandering exponent (i.e., the mean squared displacement) of the activity 13,20.

Examples of this collapse are presented in Panels A and B of Fig. 3. Considering the number of an inverted parabola, as in other systems previously studied using this method. The best fit disagrees with the empirical functions near their peak, the latter being flatter, likely an effect related to saturation observed in long events.


Figure 4: Scaling is absent in a null model resulting from defining events after randomly reordering the time series x( n). Panel A: Density distributions (CCDF) for event duration, size, and waiting time. All the distributions are exponential (note the logarithmic-linear scale). Panel B: The expected average size for a given duration in the null model is a linear function of T (the dashed line represents the fit with slope 1), therefore, j = 0 and there is no collapse.

Finally, we turn to discuss simple null models. We consider two extreme cases, in both of them the raw time series are randomly shuffled to remove serial correlations. In the first case, we remove all temporal correlations by randomly reordering x( n), thus attaining a flat power spectral density. After repeating the above analysis in this surrogate data set, it becomes clear (as shown in Fig. 4) that the scale invariance is absent in all the statistics under study: size S, waiting time Wt and duration T of events (note that the distributions are here plotted using a logarithmic-linear scale). Results in Panel B show that j +1 = 1, thus j = 0, implying that there is not collapse, because with TM = 1 in Eq. (5), the amplitude of the individual events remains invariable. To consider the second case, we need first to reorder randomly the time series of increments I( n) and then proceed to integrate the increments. Since each increment is now a random variable, the power spectral density for this surrogate process obeys f ^ with // = -2 , and as shown analytically by Baldassari et al. 13, for this case j =1/2 and the scaling function is a semicircle. Please note that the fluctuations of human activity described here differ from a simple auto-regressive process: indeed successive increments I( n) are anti-correlated and the power spectral density corresponds to non-trivial power law correlations

events here averaged (in the order of N ~ 102), the data collapse is quite satisfactory, while the value of the exponent (j = 0.48) does not exactly match the one predicted in Eq. (4), j = 0.59 (likely a consequence of insufficient sampling). To determine the generality of our results, we extended this analysis to six other data sets. For each data set, the value of j was first determined. Subsequently, the x(T, t) obtained from the events were rescaled with TM and their average computed. To account for individual differences in mean activity, shape functions were normalized by their mean value. The results for the six datasets are presented in Panel C of Fig. 3. They can be accurately described by

IV. Discussion

The present findings can be summarized by six stylized facts describing bursts of human activity: I) the spectral density of the time series of activity x(t) obeys a power law, with exponent ß ~ 1;

II) successive increments I(n) are anti-correlated with a spectral density obeying a power law with exponent 7 ~ 1, which corresponds to a spectral density for the raw data fß with ß--1;

III) the PDF of the increments I(n) is definitely non-gaussian; IV) the PDF of duration and sizes of events obeys truncated power laws with exponents 1 < t < 2 and 1 < a < 2; V) the average size of the events scales with its lifetime T as (S)(T) - T^, where j + 1 = (a - 1)/(t - 1); VI) the time series of individual events can be appropriately rescaled via a transformation of its duration T and amplitude x( t) onto a unique functional shape:

We are aware that these observations are novel only for human activity, because similar statistical regularities of avalanching activity are well known for a large variety of inanimate systems 9-12. The rescaling of the average shape is not surprising because, placed in the appropriate context, it can be traced back to Mandelbrot's study of the fractal properties of self-affine functions 21. A curve or a time series are said to be self-affine if a transformation can be found, such that rescaling their x, y coordinates by k and kM, respectively, and the variance in y is preserved (with j =1 corresponding to self-similarity). In that sense, the successful collapse of the events shape is a trivial consequence of the overall self-affinity of the x(t) time series.

Thus, it is clear that the existence of the scaling uncovered here is not informative per se of the type of mechanism behind: scale-invariance can be constructed via different processes, ranging from critical phenomena 15 to simple stochastic autoregressive dynamics 13, 20. What is then the mechanism by which the above six facts are generated?

It seems that this question cannot be easily answered by the type of experiments reported here. Fluctuations of this type could have either an intrinsic (i.e., brain-born) origin but also could be the reflection of a collective phenomena (including humans and its environment). In either case, the correlations observed seem to reject the case of independent random events starting and stopping human actions, because neither the distribution of the increments I( n), nor the exponents match the case of a random walk. In terms of brain-born process, it is hard to accept some of the implications of the scaling function in the activity shape. The average parabolic shape means that the very beginning of the motion activity contains information about how long the activity will last, in the same sense that the initial trajectory of a projectile predicts when and where it will land. This proposal is hardly realistic, because there is hardly a reasonable physiological argument in support of any motor planning for the length of time we are observing (- 103 secs). In terms of collective processes, the results here suggest that the interaction with other humans could determine when and where, on the average, we start and stop moving.

Despite our current relative ignorance, a possibility that sounds interesting is to determine in children, as they grow, if their behavioral product of parental (and otherwise) education are reflected in the shape of their individual scaling function. This seems reasonable given the fact that "tireless running around" is almost a definition of early age well-being, which gives way to less hectic activity as children mature. In the same line of thoughts, if changes in the scaling function can be quantitatively traced to behavioral changes, one could also consider to explore applications of these techniques to monitor eventual progress in the treatment of hyperactivity disturbances such as in the subjects affected by the Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder syndrome. The converse, i.e., cases in which the average activity diminish, as in elderly subjects shall be also explored. Further experiments and analysis should shed light on these possibilities. In, the meantime, the present results provide a guide and six important constraints for the models that should best capture the physics (and biology) of the process.

 

Acknowledgements - Work supported by National Science Center of Poland (ncn.gov.pl, grant DEC-2011/02/A/ST1/00119); State Secretary for Research and Development (grants PSI2010-19372 and PSI2013-48260) from Spain and by CONICET from Argentina.)

1 T Nakamura, K Kiyono, K Yoshiuchi, R Naka-hara, Z R Struzik, Y Yamamoto, Universal scaling law in human behavioral organization, Phys. Rev. Lett. 99, 138103 (2007).         [ Links ]

2 T Nakamura, et al., Of mice and men - universality and breakdown of behavioral organization, PLoS ONE 3, e2050 (2008).

3 K Hu, P C Ivanov, Z Chen, M F Hilton, H E Stanley, S A Shea, Non-random fluctuations and multi-scale dynamics regulation of human activity, Physica A 337, 307 (2004).

4 LAN Amaral, D J B Soares, L R da Silva, L S Lucena, M Saito, H Kumano, N Aoyagi, Y Yamamoto, Power law temporal auto-correlations in day-long records of human physical activity and their alteration with disease, Europhys. Lett. 66, 448 (2004).

5 C Anteneodo, D R Chialvo, Unravelling the fluctuations of animal motor activity, Chaos 19, 033123 (2009).

6 K Christensen, D Papavassiliou, A de Figueiredo, N R Franks, A B Sendova-Franks, Universality in ant behaviour, J. R. Soc. Interface 12, 20140985 (2014).

7 A Proekt, J Banavar, A Maritan, D Pfaff, Scale invariance in the dynamics of spontaneous behavior, Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109, 10564 (2012).

8 J K Ochab, et al., Scale-free fluctuations in behavioral performance: Delineating changes in spontaneous behavior of humans with induced sleep deficiency, PLoS ONE 9, e107542 (2014).

9 L Laurson, X Illa, S Santucci, K T Tallak-stad, K J Maloy, M J Alava, Evolution of the average avalanche shape with the universality class, Nature Comm. 4, 2927 (2013).

10 S Papanikolaou, F Bohn, R L Sommer, G Durin, S Zapperi, J P Sethna, Universality beyond power laws and the average avalanche shape, Nature Phys. 7, 316 (2011).

11 J P Sethna, K A Dahmen, C R Myers, Crackling noise, Nature 410, 242 (2001).

12 N Friedman, S Ito, B A W Brinkman, M Shimono, R E L DeVille, K A Dahmen, J M Beggs, T C Butler, Universal critical dynamics in high resolution neuronal avalanche data, Phys. Rev. Lett. 108, 208102 (2012).

13 A Baldassarri, F Colaiori, C Castellano, Average shape of a fluctuation: Universality in excursions of stochastic processes, Phys. Rev. Lett. 90, 060601 (2003).

14 B D Malamud, D L Turcotte, Self-affine time series: I. Generation and analyses, Adv. Geo- phys. 40, 1 (1999).

15 P Bak. How nature works. The science of self-organized criticality, Copernicus, New York (1996).

16 B Gutenberg, C F Richter, Magnitude and energy of earthquakes, Ann. Geofis. 9, 1 (1956).

17 B Wang, J Kuo, S Granick, Burst of active transport in living cells, Phys. Rev. Lett. 111, 208102 (2013).

18 L Laurson, M J Alava, 1/f noise and avalanche scaling in plastic deformation, Phys. Rev. E 74, 066106 (2006).

19 A Clauset, C R Shalizi, M E J. Newman, Power-law distributions in empirical data, SIAM Rev. 51, 661 (2009).

20 F Colaiori, A Baldassarri, C Castellano, Average trajectory of returning walks, Phys. Rev. E 69, 041105 (2004).

21 B B Mandelbrot, Self-affine fractals and fractal dimension, Physica Scripta 32, 257 (1985).

Creative Commons License Todo el contenido de esta revista, excepto dónde está identificado, está bajo una Licencia Creative Commons